DOI QR코드

DOI QR Code

Effects of Emotional Information on Visual Perception and Working Memory in Biological Motion

정서 정보가 생물형운동자극의 시지각 및 작업기억에 미치는 영향

  • Lee, Hannah (Department of Psychology, Duksung Women's University) ;
  • Kim, Jejoong (Department of Psychology, Duksung Women's University)
  • 이한나 (덕성여자대학교 심리학과) ;
  • 김제중 (덕성여자대학교 심리학과)
  • Received : 2018.09.11
  • Accepted : 2018.09.29
  • Published : 2018.09.30

Abstract

The appropriate interpretation of social cues is a crucial ability for everyday life. While processing socially relevant information, beyond the low-level physical features of the stimuli to emotional information is known to influence human cognition in various stages, from early perception to later high-level cognition, such as working memory (WM). However, it remains unclear how the influence of each type of emotional information on cognitive processes changes in response to what has occurred in the processing stage. Past studies have largely adopted face stimuli to address this type of research question, but we used a unique class of socially relevant motion stimuli, called biological motion (BM), which depicts various human actions and emotions with moving dots to exhibit the effects of anger, happiness, and neutral emotion on task performance in perceptual and working memory. In this study, participants determined whether two BM stimuli, sequentially presented with a delay between them (WM task) or one immediately after the other (perceptual task), were identical. The perceptual task showed that discrimination accuracies for emotional stimuli (i.e., angry and happy) were lower than those for neutral stimuli, implying that emotional information has a negative impact on early perceptual processes. Alternatively, the results of the WM task showed that the accuracy drop as the interstimulus interval increased was actually lower in emotional BM conditions than in the neutral condition, which suggests that emotional information benefited maintenance. Moreover, anger and happiness had distinct impacts on the performance of perception and WM. Our findings have significance as we provide evidence for the interaction of type of emotion and information-processing stage.

기존의 사회인지와 정서 연구들은 주로 얼굴자극을 이용하여 초기지각단계 및 후기인지과정에의 정서 효과를 조사해 왔다. 그러나, 정서의 효과가 정보처리과정의 각 단계에서 어떤 양상으로 나타나는지와, 정서유형에 따른 효과 양상의 변화 여부는 불확실하다. 본 연구에서는 얼굴 대신 생물형운동자극을 이용해 자극에 내포된 행복, 분노, 중립정서가 지각과제와 작업기억과제 수행에 미치는 영향을 조사하였다. 참가자는 연달아(지각과제) 또는 시간차를 두고(작업기억과제) 제시되는 두 생물형운동의 동일 여부를 판단하였다. 지각과제에서는 정서가를 가진 자극 시행의 정확도가 중립정서자극에 비해 낮아 정서정보가 초기지각처리에는 부정적 영향을 미치는 것으로 보였으나, 작업기억과제에서의 기억정보유지에는 도움이 되는 것으로 나타났다. 또한, 각 과제에서 정서유형에 따라 다른 수행 양상이 관찰되었다. 분노정서는 지각단계에서 더 많은 정신적 자원을 요구하여 부하가 증가할 경우 정확도가 낮아지지만 기억유지에는 긍정적으로 작용하는 것으로 보이며. 행복정서의 경우 중립정서가 이어서 제시될 때 이를 행복정서와 유사하게 처리하려는 편향을 유도하는 것으로 보인다. 본 연구는 생물형운동자극을 이용하여 정보처리과정에서의 정서 영향을 재확인하였고, 처리단계별 및 정서 종류별로 다른 양상의 영향이 나타난다는 점을 추가로 밝혀, 정서정보의 정교한 조작 및 통제를 위한 유용한 단서를 제공한다.

Keywords

References

  1. Allison, T., Puce, A., McCarthy, G. (2000). Social perception from visual cues: role of the STS region. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 267-278. DOI: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01501-1
  2. Atkinson, A. P. (2009). Impaired recognition of emotions from body movements is associated with elevated motion coherence threshold in autism spectrum disorders. Neuropsychologia, 47, 3023-3029. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.05.019
  3. Atkinson, A. P., Dittrich, W. H., Gemmell, A. J., & Young, A. W. (2004). Emotion perception from dynamic and static body expressions in point-light and full-light displays. Perception, 33, 717-746. DOI: 10.1068/p5096
  4. Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: Bradford/MIT press.
  5. Basso, M. R., Schefft, B. K., Ris, M. D., & Dember, W. N. (1996). Mood and global-local visual processing. Journal of International Neuropsychological Society, 2, 249-255. DOI: 10.1017/S1355617700001193.
  6. Becker, D. V., Anderson, U. S., Mortensen, C. R., Neufeld, S. L., & Neel, R. (2011). The face in the crowd effect unconfounded: Happy faces, not angry faces, are more efficiently detected in single- and multiple-target visual search tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140(4), 637-659. DOI:10.1037/a0024060
  7. Becker, D. V., Neel, R., Srinivasan, N., Neufeld, S., Kumar, D., & Fouse, S. (2012). The vividness of happiness in dynamic facial displays of emotion. PLoS ONE, 7(1), e26551. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0026551
  8. Brainard, D. H. (1997). The psychophysics toolbox. Spatial Vision, 10, 443-446. DOI: 10.1163/156856897X00357
  9. Ceccarini, F., & Caudek, C. (2013). Anger superiority effect: The importance of dynamic emotional facial expressions. Visual Cognition, 21(4), 498-540, DOI: 10.1080/13506285.2013.807901.
  10. Chouchourelou, A., Matsuka, T., Harber, K., & Shiffrar, M. (2006). The visual analysis of emotional actions. Social Neuroscience, 1, 63-74. DOI: 10.1080/17470910600630599
  11. Clarke, T. J., Bradshaw, M. F., Field, D. T., Hampson, S. E., & Rose, D. (2005). The perception of emotion from body movement in point-light displays of interpersonal dialogue. Perception, 34, 1171-1180. DOI: 10.1068/p5203
  12. Corcoran, R., Mercer, G., & Frith, C. D. (1995). Schizophrenia, symptomatology and social inference: investigating "theory of mind" in people with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 17, 5-13. DOI: 10.1016/0920-9964(95)00024-G
  13. Craig, B. M., Becker, S. I., & Lipp, O. V. (2014). Different faces in the crowd: A happiness superiority effect for schematic faces in heterogeneous background. Emotion, 14(4), 794-803. DOI:10.1037/a0036043
  14. Diener, E., & Diener, C. (1996). Most people are happy. Psychological Science, 7, 181-185. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1996.tb00354.x
  15. Ekman, P., & Friesen, W.V. (1984). Unmasking the face. Palo Alto, Calif: Consulting Psychology Press.
  16. Fine, M. S., & Minnery, B. S. (2009). Visual salience affects performance in a working memory task. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 8016-8021. DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5503-08.2009
  17. Frith, C. D., & Corcoran, R. (1996). Exploring "theory of mind" in people with schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine, 26, 521-530. DOI:10.1017/S0033291700035601
  18. Gaebel, W., & Wolwer W. (1992). Facial expression and emotional face recognition in schizophrenia and depression. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 242, 46-52. DOI: 10.1007/BF02190342
  19. Gasper, K., & Clore, G. L. (2002). Attending to the big picture: mood and global versus local processing of visual information. Psychological Science, 13(1), 34-40, DOI: 10.1111/1467-9280.00406
  20. Grady, C. L., & Keightley, M. L. (2002). Studies of altered social cognition in neuropsychiatric disorders using functional neuroimaging. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 47, 327-336. DOI:10.1177/070674370204700403
  21. Hahn, S., & Gronlund, S. D. (2007). Top-down guidance in visual search for facial expressions. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14(1), 159-165. DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00155
  22. Hansen, C. H., & Hansen, R. D. (1988). Finding the face in the crowd: An anger superiority effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 917-924. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.917
  23. Hubert, B., Wicker, B., Moore, D. G., Monfardini, E., Duverger, H., Da Fonseca D., & Deruelle, C. (2007). Brief report: recognition of emotional and non-emotional biological motion in individuals with autistic spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 1386-1392. DOI: 10.1007/s10803-007-0378-0
  24. Jackson, M. C., Linden, D. E., & Raymond, J. E. (2014). Angry expressions strengthen the encoding and maintenance of face identity representations in visual working memory. Cognition & Emotion, 28(2), 278-297. DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2013.816655
  25. Jackson, M. C., Wolf, C., Johnston, S. J., Raymond, J. E., & Linden, D. E. (2008). Neural correlates of enhanced visual short-term memory for angry faces: An fMRI study. PLoS ONE, 3(10): e3536, DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0003536
  26. Johansson, G. (1973). Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis. Perception and Psychophysics, 14, 201-211. DOI: 10.3758/BF03212378
  27. Kensinger, E. A., & Corkin, S. (2003). Effect of negative emotional content on working memory and longterm memory. Emotion, 3(4), 378-393. DOI: 10.1037/1528-3542.3.4.378
  28. Kim, J. (2012). Biological motion: Perceptual processing, neural mechanisms and clinical application. The Korean Journal of Cognitive and Biological Psychology, 24(4), 357-392. https://doi.org/10.22172/cogbio.2012.24.4.004
  29. LeDoux, J. E. (1995). Emotion: clues from the brain. Annual Reviews of Psychology, 46, 209-235. DOI: 10.1146/annurev.ps.46.020195.001233
  30. Lee, H., & Kim, J. (2017). Facilitating effects of emotion on the perception of biological motion: Evidence for a happiness superiority effect. Perception, 46(6), 679-697. DOI:10.1177/0301006616681809
  31. Lyyra, P., Hietanen, J. K., & Astikainen, P. (2014). Anger superiority effect for change detection and change blindness. Consciousness and Cognition, 30, 1-12, DOI: 10.1016/j.concog.2014.07.013.
  32. Ma, Y., Paterson, H.M., & Pollick, F.E. (2006). A motion-capture library for the study of identity, gender, and emotion perception from biological motion. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 38, 134-141. DOI: 10.3758/BF03192758
  33. Manera, V., Schouten, B., Becchio, C., Bara, B. G., & Verfaillie, K. (2010). Inferring intentions from biological motion: a stimulus set of point-light communicative interactions. Behavior Research Methods, 42(1), 168-178. DOI: 10.3758/BRM.42.1.168.
  34. Mayer, J. S., Kim, J., & Park, S. (2011). Enhancing visual working memory encoding: The role of target novelty. Visual Cognition, 19(7), 863-885. DOI: 10.1080/13506285.2011.594459
  35. McHugo, M., Olatunji, B. O., & Zald, D. H. (2013). The emotional attentional blink: what we know so far. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7. DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00151
  36. Mermillod, M., Vermeulen, N., Lundqvist, D., & Niedenthal, P. M. (2009). Neural computation as a tool to differentiate perceptual from emotional processes: the case of anger superiority effect. Cognition, 110(3), 346-357. DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2008.11.009
  37. Okruszek, L. (2018). It is not just in faces! Processing of emotion and intention from biological motion in psychiatric disorders. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 12, 48 DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00048.
  38. Osaka, M., Yaoi, K., Minamoto, T., & Osaka, N. (2013). When do negative and positive emotions modulate working memory performance? Scientific Reports, 3, 1375. DOI:10.1038/srep01375
  39. Palermo, R., & Rhodes, G., (2007). Are you always on my mind? A review of how face perception and attention interact. Neuropsychologia, 45, 75-92. DOI:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.04.025
  40. Pelli, D. G. (1997). The video toolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. Spatial Vision, 10, 437-442. DOI: 10.1163/156856897X00366
  41. Phillips, M. L., & David, A. S. (1997). Viewing strategies for simple and chimeric faces: An investigation of perceptual bias in normals and schizophrenic patients using visual scan paths. Brain and Cognition, 35, 225-238. DOI: 10.1006/brcg.1997.0939
  42. Roesch, E., B., Sander, D., Mumenthaler, C., Kerzel, D., & Scherer, K. R. (2010). Psychophysics of emotion: The QUEST for emotional attention. Journal of Vision, 104, 1-9. DOI:10.1167/10.3.4.
  43. Schmidt, B. K., Vogel, E. K., Woodman, G. F., & Luck, S. J. (2002). Voluntary and automatic attentional control of visual working memory. Perception and Psychophysics, 64, 754-763. DOI: 10.1037/cep0000029
  44. Svegar, D., Kardum, I., & Polic, M. (2013). Happy face superiority effect in change detection paradigm. Psychological Topics, 22(2), 249-269.
  45. Treisman, A. M., & Gelade, G. (1980). A featureintegration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 97-136. DOI:10.1016/0010-0285(80)90005-5
  46. van Boxtel, J. J. A., & Lu, H. (2013). A biological motion toolbox for reading, displaying, and manipulating motion capture data in research settings. Journal of Vision, 13(12), 7, 1-16. DOI: 10.1167/13.12.7
  47. Zadra, J. R., & Clore, G. L. (2011). Emotion and perception: The role of affective information. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 2, 676-685. DOI:10.1002/wcs.147